touchOPHTHALMOLOGY touchOPHTHALMOLOGY
Anterior Segment, Corneal and External Disorders
Read Time: 4 mins

Response to “Optical Lens Tinting—A Review of its Functional Mechanism, Efficacy, and Applications”

Copy Link
Published Online: Jun 11th 2020 US Ophthalmic Review. 2020;13(1):16–7 DOI: https://doi.org/10.17925/USOR.2020.13.1.16
Authors: Arnold J Wilkins
Quick Links:
Abstract
Article
Article Information
Abstract:
Overview

Although the FL–41 lens was originally designed to reduce flicker, it rarely does so now because of changes in lamp and driver technology. If it continues to be effective in reducing photophobia it may be for reasons that are complex and relate to cortical excitability.

Keywords

Optical lens tinting, FL–41 lens, photophobia

Article:

I was interested to read the review, “Optical Lens Tinting—A Review of its Functional Mechanism, Efficacy, and Applications” by Jared Raabe, Ashwini Kini, and Andrew Lee, which appeared in US Ophthalmic Review.1

I am the inventor of the FL–41 lens that features in the article. As the authors point out, the original design of the lens was an attempt to reduce the flicker from fluorescent lamps in the days when most were operated from magnetic circuitry. The lamps then had a halophosphate coating, a phosphor that converted the ultraviolet radiation from the gas discharge to long-wavelength light. The coating continued to emit light after excitation by the discharge and, as a result, the light from the lamps flickered less at the long-wavelength end of the visible spectrum than at the short.2 The gas discharge occurred twice with each cycle of the alternating current electricity supply. This (100 Hz) flicker was too fast to be seen, but was shown to be responsible for headaches in office workers in a double-masked study that compared magnetic circuitry with electronic circuitry, which reduced the 100-Hz flicker.3

The FL–41 tint had a low transmission of short-wavelength light and thereby also reduced the flicker. Good and Mortimer used the tint with school children and compared it with a blue tint, which was less effective in reducing headaches.4 The reason for the reduction in headaches from the FL–41 was most probably the school lighting, which used halophosphate fluorescent lamps operated from magnetic circuitry.5

Later, the phosphors in fluorescent lamps were changed for more efficient television phosphors and it was no longer possible to reduce the flicker much by using spectral filters. Later still,
high-frequency electronic ballasts became widespread and the problem of flicker from fluorescent lighting was solved.  Unfortunately, it is now re-emerging with light-emitting diode (LED) lighting, but that is another story.

When I had a laboratory in which the fluorescent lamp could be switched between electronic circuitry (flicker-free) and magnetic circuitry (100-Hz flicker), I was asked to see a patient with blepharospasm. She was free of spasm until the lighting was switched to magnetic circuitry, and the spasm then continued when the lighting was switched back to electronic. The FL–41 tint has been shown to reduce blepharospasm, and I therefore wonder whether invisible flicker from fluorescent lighting is partly responsible.6

I have no commercial interest in the FL–41 tint, but it continues to be sold in various guises by several companies on the internet. As Raabe et al. point out, one possible reason is that the tint reduces the stimulation of the intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells, which have been linked to photophobia and are most sensitive at the short-wavelength end of the visible spectrum where the FL–41 transmission is lowest.

However, I would argue that the evidence for a selective role of melanopsin in photophobia is not strong, at least as regards the photophobia that accompanies migraine. Following the original reports by Noseda, Burstein et al., other papers by these authors have shown less exacerbation of pain during headache from green light not red, and subsequently the same authors have implicated the rods.7–9 In a recent review in Cephalalgia, Noseda et al. postulate “abnormal processing of light by both cone/rod-mediated image-forming and melanopsin non-image-forming visual pathways.”10

So I wonder why the FL–41 continues to be effective in reducing photophobia. Of course, there remains a large number of fluorescent lamps controlled by magnetic ballasts, with television phosphors that fluctuate in brightness and chromaticity at twice the frequency of the electricity supply.2 Even 120-Hz flicker is well within the frequency sensitivity range of the retinal cells (electroretinogram signals attenuate above 200 Hz), so a colored lens might reduce some of the chromaticity fluctuation, and this might reduce fatigue.11

However, I think there may be another explanation relating to cortical hyper-excitability, as in the paper by Huang et al.12 Photophobia in migraine is often conceived as a sensitivity to bright light, but it is so much more. Patients with photophobic migraine have an aversion to flicker, and to patterns, particularly patterns of stripes (those that are epileptogenic in patients with photosensitive epilepsy).13,14 If the photophobia is a symptom of a cortical hyper-excitability, which is common in migraine, then thehyper-excitability is unlikely to be uniform throughout the cortex (even in patients with photosensitive epilepsy, the excitability can involve only neurons with particular orientation selectivity).14–16 The limited knowledge we have of cortical processing of color suggests that in V2 the cells are arranged as per a map of color rather similar to the International Commission on Illumination Uniform Chromaticity Scale (CIE UCS) diagram.17

In recent work, we find that when asked to choose lighting comfortable for reading, most individuals choose a color of light similar to that which they would experience from everyday light sources, both natural (sky blue) and artificial (white or yellow). Patients who experience migraine with aura, on the other hand, choose a light far more saturated in color than is typical of conventional sources of light.18 We hypothesize that the color redistributes excitation in the cortex so as to avoid local patches of hyper-excitability. There are large individual differences in the color chosen, and the choice of color provides little empirical support for melanopsin as a source of photophobia.18

In conclusion, the causes of photophobia in migraine involve not only bright light, but flickering light and stressful patterns. Flickering light and stressful patterns are unnatural, uncomfortable, and a common feature of the modern urban environment.19–22 

Article Information:
Disclosure

Arnold J Wilkins has no financial or non-financial relationships or activities to declare in relation to this article.

Review Process

This article is a short commentary piece; it has not been submitted to external peer reviewers, but was reviewed by the Editorial Board for relevance before publication.

Authorship

The named author meets the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (ICMJE) criteria for authorship of this manuscript, takes responsibility for the integrity of the work as a whole, and has given final approval for the version to be published.

Correspondence

Arnold J Wilkins,
Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Colchester, CO4 3SQ, UK. E: arnold@essex.ac.uk

Support

No funding was received in the publication of this article.

Access

This article is freely accessible at touchOPHTHALMOLOGY.com ©Touch Medical Media 2020

Received

2020-03-31

References

  1. Raabe J, Kini A, Lee A. Optical lens tinting—a review of its functional mechanism, efficacy, and applications. US Ophthalmic Rev. 2019;12:85–7.
  2. Wilkins AJ, Clark C. Modulation of light from fluorescent lamps. Light Res Technol. 1990;22:103–9.
  3. Wilkins AJ, Nimmo-Smith I, Slater AI, Bedocs L. Fluorescent lighting, headaches and eyestrain. Light Res Technol. 1989;21:11–8.
  4. Good PA, Taylor RH, Mortimer MJ. The use of tinted glasses in childhood migraine. Headache. 1991;31:533–6.
  5. Winterbottom M, Wilkins A. Lighting and discomfort in the classroom. J Environ Psychol. 2009;29:63–75.
  6. Blackburn MK, Lamb RD, Digre KB, et al. FL-41 tint improves blink frequency, light sensitivity, and functional limitations in patients with benign essential blepharospasm. Ophthalmology. 2009;116:997–1001.
  7. Noseda R, Kainz V, Jakubowski M, et al. A neural mechanism for exacerbation of headache by light. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13: 239–45.
  8. Noseda R, Bernstein CA, Nir R-R, et al. Migraine photophobia originating in cone-driven retinal pathways. Brain. 2016; 139:1971–86.
  9. Bernstein CA, Nir R-R, Noseda R, et al. The migraine eye: distinct rod-driven retinal pathways’ response to dim light challenges the visual cortex hyperexcitability theory. Pain. 2018;160:569–78.
  10. Noseda R, Copenhagen D, Burstein R. Current understanding of photophobia, visual networks and headaches. Cephalalgia. 2019;39:1623–34.
  11. Berman SM, Greenhouse DS, Bailey IL, et al. Human electroretinogram responses to video displays, fluorescent lighting, and other high frequency sources. Optom Vis Sci. 1991;68:64562.
  12. Huang J, Zong X, Wilkins A, et al. FMRI evidence that precision ophthalmic tints reduce cortical hyperactivation in migraine. Cephalalgia. 2011;31:925–36.
  13. Wilkins A, Nimmo-Smith I, Tait A, et al. A neurological basis for visual discomfort. Brain. 1984;107:989–1017.
  14. Wilkins AJ, Darby CE, Binnie CD. Neurophysiological aspects of pattern-sensitive epilepsy. Brain. 1979;102:1–25.
  15. Wilkins AJ, Hibbard PB. Discomfort and hypermetabolism. Conference paper presented at: 50th Annual Convention of the Society for Artificial Intelligence and Simullation of Behaviour (AISB), April 1–4, 2014, Goldsmiths, University of London, UK. Available at: www.researchgate.net/publication/261724201_Discomfort_and_hypermetabolism/ (accessed June 4, 2020).
  16. Welch KM, D’Andrea G, Tepley N, et al. The concept of migraine as a state of central neuronal hyperexcitability. Neurol Clin. 1990;8:817–28.
  17. Xiao Y, Wang Y, Felleman DJ. A spatially organized representation of colour in macaque cotical area V2. Nature. 2003;421:535–9.
  18. Vieira A, van der Linde I, Bright P, Wilkins A. Preference for lighting chromaticity in migraine with aura. Headache. 2020;60:1124–31.
  19. Brown E, Foulsham T, Lee C, Wilkins AJ. Visibility of temporal light artefact from flicker at 11 kHz. Light Res Technol. 2020;52: 371–6.
  20. Le ATD, Payne J, Clarke C. et al. Discomfort from urban scenes: metabolic consequences. Landsc Urban Plan. 2017;160:61–8.
  21. Wilkins AJ, Penacchio O, Leonards U. The built environment and its patterns: a view from the vision sciences. J Sustain Des Appl Res. 2018;6. Available at: https://arrow.tudublin.ie/sdar/vol6/iss1/5/ (accessed June 8, 2020).
  22. Wilkins A, Smith K, Penacchio O. The influence of typography on algorithms that predict the speed and comfort of reading. Vision (Basel). 2020;4:18.

Further Resources

Share this Article
Related Content In Corneal and External Disorders
  • Copied to clipboard!
    accredited arrow-down-editablearrow-downarrow_leftarrow-right-bluearrow-right-dark-bluearrow-right-greenarrow-right-greyarrow-right-orangearrow-right-whitearrow-right-bluearrow-up-orangeavatarcalendarchevron-down consultant-pathologist-nurseconsultant-pathologistcrosscrossdownloademailexclaimationfeedbackfiltergraph-arrowinterviewslinkmdt_iconmenumore_dots nurse-consultantpadlock patient-advocate-pathologistpatient-consultantpatientperson pharmacist-nurseplay_buttonplay-colour-tmcplay-colourAsset 1podcastprinter scenerysearch share single-doctor social_facebooksocial_googleplussocial_instagramsocial_linkedin_altsocial_linkedin_altsocial_pinterestlogo-twitter-glyph-32social_youtubeshape-star (1)tick-bluetick-orangetick-red tick-whiteticktimetranscriptup-arrowwebinar Sponsored Department Location NEW TMM Corporate Services Icons-07NEW TMM Corporate Services Icons-08NEW TMM Corporate Services Icons-09NEW TMM Corporate Services Icons-10NEW TMM Corporate Services Icons-11NEW TMM Corporate Services Icons-12Salary £ TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-01TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-02TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-03TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-04TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-05TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-06TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-07TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-08TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-09TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-10TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-11TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-12TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-13TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-14TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-15TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-16TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-17TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-18TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-19TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-20TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-21TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-22TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-23TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-24TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-25TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-26TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-27TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-28TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-29TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-30TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-31TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-32TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-33TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-34TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-35TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-36TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-37TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-38TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-39TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-40TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-41TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-42TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-43TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-44TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-45TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-46TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-47TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-48TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-49TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-50TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-51TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-52TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-53TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-54TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-55TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-56TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-57TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-58TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-59TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-60TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-61TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-62TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-63TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-64TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-65TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-66TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-67TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-68TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-69TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-70TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-71TMM-Corp-Site-Icons-72